J84
Tohoku J. Exp. Med., 2006,
209,B e
2 a0l Effects of Fluvastatin in Rats
Fluvastatin Alters Psychomotor Performance and Daily
Activity but not the Spatial Memory in Rats
SUKRUCAN H. BAYTAN, MEHMET ALKANAT, MEHMET OZEREN,1 MURAT EKINCI2 and
AHMET AKGUN
Department of Physiology, 1Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology,
Karadeniz Technical University, Medical School, Trabzon, Turkey, and
2Department of Computer Engineering, Engineering School, Karadeniz
Technical University, Trabzon, Turkey
BAYTAN, S.H., ALKANAT, M., OZEREN, M., EKINCI, M. and AKGUN, A.
Fluvastatin
Alters Psychomotor Performance and Daily Activity but not the Spatial Memory in Rats.
Tohoku J. Exp. Med., 2006,
209 (4), 311-320
Statins, inhibitors of cholesterol synthe-
sis for treating dyslipidemia and preventing cardiovascular complications, have been
shown to alter central nervous system functions. Our aim was to investigate the effects of
the fluvastatin, a member of statin family, on psychomotor performance, daily activity and
spatial memory. Sprague-Dawley rats were treated with fluvastatin (
n = 8) or placebo as a
control (
n = 11) regardless of sex. Fluvastatin (7.5 mg/kg) was administered orally once a
day for four weeks, while the control group was administered only placebo. Psychomotor
performance was measured by rotarod tests. No significant difference was observed in the
fluvastatin group over the course of weeks, but the control group preferred to stay on the
device shorter times (
p < 0.05). For the first three weeks of the drug administration there
was a statistical difference between the groups, however no difference was found after the
4th week. There was no difference in the Barnes maze spatial memory test between the
groups and also within the groups over the course of time. Daily activity tests revealed
that stereotypical and vertical movements of the fluvastatin group were significantly less
than the control group in all four weeks. Ambulatory movements and the distances taken
by the fluvastatin group were decreased significantly over the course of time (
p < 0.005
and
p < 0.001, respectively), but the control group did not reveal any significant change.
Our results suggest that fluvastatin altered psychomotor performance and daily activity in
rats, but it did not affect the spatial memory. These behavioral changes might be associat-
ed with alterations in the composition of the brain lipids caused by fluvastatin.
statin; fluvastatin; behavior; Barnes maze; psychomotor
2006 Tohoku University Medical Press
Statins, inhibitors of the 3-hydroxy-3-meth-
al. 2004). Statins change lipid metabolism, lower
ylglutaryl-CoA (HMG-CoA) reductase in the syn-
low-density lipoproteins, increase high-density
thesis of cholesterol, are widely used in the pre-
lipoproteins, and decrease tryglicerids depending
vention of cardiovascular diseases by decreasing
on specific statin used (Schaefer et al. 2004).
blood lipid levels (Maron et al. 2000; Collins et
Cholesterol metabolism and transport of
Received February 22, 2006; revision accepted for publication May 25, 2006.
Correspondence: Dr. Sukrucan H. Baytan, KTU Tip Fak, Fizyoloji ABD, 61080 Trabzon, Turkey.
S.H. Baytan et al.
cholesterol turnover within the brain were
fluvastatin on behavioral changes by evaluating
reviewed by Bjorkhem and Meaney (2004).
spatial memory, psychomotor performance and
Statin applications and lowered plasma cholester-
daily activity in Sprague-Dawley rats.
ol levels were found to affect central nervous sys-
tem (CNS) functions in humans. Severe irritabili-
ty and aggression with statin usage in six patients
General procedure
were reported by Golomb et al. (2004a, b). They
The study was approved by Karadeniz Technical
also addressed that there is a need for more data
University, Medical School animal ethics committee
to establish the impact of hydrophilic and lipo-
(12-October-2005 ref: 473).
philic statins on cognition, aggression, and brain
Twelve male and twelve female Sprague-Dawley
serotonin activity. Cultured glial cel s have HMG-
rats with initial weights of 200-240 g, 5-6 months old
CoA reductase as the major rate-limiting enzyme
were provided and maintained in our vivarium (constant
for their synthesis of cholesterol (Snipes and Suter
temperature of 23 ± 1°C; 60 ± 10% relative humidity;
1997). A well known fact is that myelination is
12-h/12-h light cycle; food and water ad lib.). The ani-
preceded by increased esterification of cholesterol,
mals were kept in our vivarium for two weeks for habitu-
which is possibly a mechanism for accumulation
ation without any testing and training. The animals were
of the required very high amounts of cholesterol
divided into two groups (6 males and 6 females in each
in the developing CNS (Norton and Cammer
group total of 12 animals in each group) as control and
1984). Experimental evidence for a cholesterol
fluvastatin groups using the random table. Fluvastatin
shuttle from astrocytes to neurons in an in vitro
(LescolTM, Novartis, Basel, Switzerland) was obtained
commercially and homogenized in phosphate buffered
system had been presented by Mauch et al. (2001).
saline (PBS). The control group was administered the
Benefical effects of atorvastatin on human higher
PBS solution without the drug as placebo. The adminis-
cortical functions were studied by Parale et al.
tered solution volume each time was 0.5 ml in both
(2006), which lays strong foundations for future
groups. Fluvastatin (7.5 mg/kg once a day) and placebo
human studies about mental activities and statin
was administered for a total of 28 days orally by gavage
usage relationships. These behavioral studies
around 8:30 am. Testing was designed as all measure-
clearly showed that statin treatment can alter
ments were taken once a week on the 0th, 7th, 14th, 21st,
behavior with its possible cholesterol altering
and 28th days, regardless of sex. Five animals had to be
effects in the CNS.
excluded from the study during the testing period, two
The effects of several statins on lipid compo-
animals died from unknown causes, two animals repeat-
sition of the brain in rats, including fluvastatin,
edly failed to find the goal box in the Barnes maze, and
were observed by Vecka et al. (2004), in which
one animal was observed to be pregnant. With the
fluvastatin, an important member of the statin
excluded animals, the fluvastatin group consisted of 8
family, was shown to increase sphingomyelin and
animals (4 males and 4 females). The control group con-
to decrease diphosphatidylglycerol contents in rat
sisted of 11 (5 males and 6 females) animals for
brains. Yet, the most important changes in the
fatty acid profile being in the ceramide monohex-
osides, lead to a decrease in the content of saturat-
Rotarod test (Psychomotor performance)
ed fatty acids and an increase in the content of
An accelerating rotarod device designed for mice
and rats was used for the psychomotor performance eval-
polyunsaturated fatty acids. Moreover, it has
uation. The rod was 4.5 cm in diameter, covered with
been shown that chronic administration of statins
non-slippery material, 30 cm above the device floor
alters multiple gene expression patterns in the
(Commat Inc., Ankara, Turkey). The animals were first
mouse cerebral cortex (Johnson-Anuna et al.
habituated on the rod and trained for 5 days before test-
2005). From these studies' standpoints, changes
ings had started. Each training day the animals were
in CNS functions and behaviors could be expect-
expected to stay on the rod as long as they could. After
ed with statin treatment.
the first fall, it was taken to its homecage to rest for at
In this study, we investigated the effects of
least 15 min and then the second trial was started. Every
Behavioral Effects of Fluvastatin in Rats
animal took three trials per day during the training peri-
the escape hole was covered by a light tight material and
od. The training trials started with the stationary rod for
the animal was allowed to stay there for 60 sec. The pro-
60 sec, and then it started to revolve and gradually
cedure was repeated four times during each testing peri-
increase to 16 rpm. There was no punishment system
od. If an animal could not find the hole in 5 min, an
that was used on the rotarod tests. After the training
experimenter gently drove the animal to the escape hole
period and one day resting period, the testing phase was
and the animal was allowed to rest there for 60 sec. In
started. In the testing phase the same procedure was
the training and testing trials, the goal box position
applied, except each animal underwent 5 trials in each
remained the same between measurements, but maze
experiment day. Latency times are measured in seconds.
holes were turned randomly each time for animals to
avoid following odor trails. Before and after each test
Barnes maze test (Spatial memory and reference learn-
and training session, the maze was wiped clean for the
same purpose as well. The animals were also trained
A modified Barnes maze was utilized to perform
once before each experiment day and the hole position
spatial learning and reference memory tests described by
was changed for each testing week. In the first training
Barnes (1979) and Turner et al. (2004). The maze was
trial, animals did not know the position of the goal box.
122 cm in diameter made of water resistant white materi-
In the testing period, the hole remained in the same posi-
al, had 12 holes with a diameter of 9.5 cm equidistant
tion. The testing procedure was the same as in the train-
from each other, and 3 cm away from the perimeter.
ing period, but when the start box was removed, a timer
Height of the maze was 140 cm to discourage animals
was started. Head pokes or closely approaching and
from jumping to the floor. A 10 cm wide, 20 cm long,
visually checking the wrong holes by the animals were
and 12 cm high stainless steel goal box was placed under
counted as errors. Repeated pokes in the same hole or
one hole during the training and experiment sessions.
several stares to a wrong hole in a single approach were
Animals were placed in a light-tight start box placed in
counted as one error. The timer stopped when all paws
the middle of the maze, equivalent to the goal box. Extra
touched the goal box. Four measurements were recorded
maze cues were placed around the maze, big round and
on each testing day.
square dark colored posters hung on the white wall, and
a divider curtain was used to hide the experimenters
Daily activity
while the tests were being performed. A floodlight was
A computerized cage system for animal activity was
used 180 cm over the maze for aversive reaction on tests.
used to assess 24-hr daily locomotor activity, such as
A video system, which was used for recording animal
ambulatory movements, vertical movements, stereotypi-
movements, was attached on top of the maze. A televi-
cal movements, and distances were taken in centimeters
sion monitor was located behind the divider curtain. The
(Commat Inc.). The cage was a clear cube (each side 42
video recordings were evaluated offline to calculate the
cm). Horizontal infrared beam rays on each four sides
distance taken by the animals. Distance measurements
(2.5 cm apart to each other, 2.5 cm in height) monitored
were calculated by using the Software developed by and
the horizontal movements. There was also a similar
the Department of Computer Science and the Department
beam design in the height of 13 cm to record vertical
of Physiology, Medical School in Karadeniz University.
movements of the animal. Stereotypical movements
Before the tests began, all animals were allowed to
such as itching, sniffing and head bobbings were record-
stay on the Barnes maze for one hour moving freely
ed. Also the numbers of horizontal movements in the
without the goal box for habituation. Afterwards, 4 train-
cage (ambulatory) were counted, while stereotypical or
ing sessions for 5 days were conducted. Each animal
vertical movements were excluded in the ambulatory
was brought to the testing laboratory separately. After a
counts. Vertical movements, and the total horizontal dis-
five minute resting period, it was placed under the start
tances taken by the animals (distance) were calculated.
box and kept there for 10 sec with their heads always
The data within the control and fluvastatin groups in
positioned to the north. Lights were turned on, experi-
the proceeding weeks were statistically evaluated with
menters moved behind the divider, and then the start box
the Friedman test. These groups were compared with
was removed remotely by a pulley system and the
each other separately every week from week zero
animals were expected to find the goal box. When the
through week four by Mann-Whitney's U-test. A
p-value
animal found the escape hole, the lights were turned off,
of smaller than 0.05 (
p < 0.05) was considered to be
S.H. Baytan et al.
significant. Statistical analysis was performed by statisti-
no difference observed after the 4th week.
cal program package, SPSS version 13.01 (Lead
Technologies Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).
Barnes maze test
There was no difference between the fluvas-
tatin and the control group for errors, latency
Rotarod test
times and distances (
p > 0.05, Table 2). Also,
The fluvastatin group showed no significant
comparisons between the fluvastatin and placebo
difference over the course of the weeks while the
groups for four weeks indicated there was no sig-
control group stayed on the rotarod device for
nificant difference between the groups.
shorter times over the course of time (
p < 0.05,
Table 1). As expected, there were no differences
Daily activity tests
observed in the 0th week. While after the 1st, 2nd
All data about daily activity tests are shown
and 3rd weeks a difference was found, there was
in the Table 3.
TABLE 1. Latency times of fluvastatin and control groups on the rotarod device §.
§ Latency times were expressed in seconds.
The Data are in mean ± S.E.M. format. *
p < 0.05.
†
p values within the groups by the Friedman test.
††
p values between the groups for each week by the Mann-Whitney's U-test.
TABLE 2. Barnes maze error counts, latency times and distances for each week in the groups §.
Distance (cm) Control
§Error counts on the Barnes maze, latency times for entering right hole in seconds, and distances taken
on the maze in centimeters.
The Data are in mean ± S.E.M. format.
†
p values within the groups by the Friedman test.
††
p values between the groups for each week by the Mann-Whitney's U-test.
Behavioral Effects of Fluvastatin in Rats
TABLE 3. Daily activity measurements for fluvastatin and control groups§.
Stereotypical Control
10,580 ± 974 11,707 ± 890 11,382 ± 1,013 11,432 ± 884 11,344 ± 914
Ambulatory Control
8,434 ± 734 8,259 ± 1,032 8,639 ± 1,209 8,939 ± 1,437 8,514 ± 1,319 0.91
5,954 ± 594 6,722 ± 1,048 0.000*
§ Counts of stereotypical, ambulatory, vertical and horizontal movements, and distances taken in
centimeters, in the daily activity cage.
The Data are in mean ± S.E.M. format. *
p < 0.05.
†
p values within the groups by the Friedman test.
††
p values between the groups for each week by the Mann-Whitney's U-test.
Stereotypical movements. During the course
found to take less distance during the course of
of time no difference was shown statistically in
time (
p < 0.001), but no change was observed in
either group. When the difference between
the control group. When the difference between
groups was analyzed each week, the fluvastatin
groups over the weeks was investigated, no statis-
group was found to make less stereotypical move-
tical difference between the groups was observed.
ments from the 1st week to 4th week.
From the results, psychomotor performance
Ambulatory movements. The fluvastatin
was found to be altered in the control group
group was found to have less ambulatory move-
during the course of the time that they ran on the
ment counts during the course of time (
p < 0.005),
rotarod device for shorter time periods. Between
but the same significance was not observed for the
the 1st and the 3rd weeks, a significant difference
control group. When the difference between the
was observed between the control and fluvastatin
groups was investigated on a weekly basis, the
groups. But, the fluvastatin group did not change
fluvastatin group was found to have less ambula-
their psychomotor performance during the course
tory movements only on the 3rd week.
of the time. Spatial memory was not affected by
Vertical movements. During the course of
fluvastatin use, according to the Barnes maze
time no difference was observed statistically in
measurements. The results of daily activity tests
either group. When the difference between the
revealed that the fluvastatin group made less
groups was investigated for each week, the fluv-
ambulatory movements and took less distances
astatin group was found to have less vertical
during the course of time. When compared to the
movements from the 1st week to 4th week.
control group, the fluvastatin group displayed
Total distance. The fluvastatin group was
stereotypical and vertical movements that were
S.H. Baytan et al.
found to be significantly less in the last four drug
with aggression (Golomb et al. 2004b).
administered weeks.
Fluvastatin is known as a lipophilic drug (De
Angelis 2004). Effects of fluvastatin in the rat
brain were shown by Vecka et al. (2004). Another
It has been suggested that statins may pro-
study reported that lovastatin and simvastatin
vide beneficial effects that are not limited to
strongly reduced the levels of free cholesterol in
reduced levels of low-density lipoproteins and tri-
synaptozomal plazma membranes and lovastatin
glycerides in the blood (Liao 2002, 2004, 2005).
and pravastatin significantly reduced cholesterol
Statins are expected to affect any organ or system
levels in the exofacial membrane leaflet. These
involved in lipid metabolism, including the CNS,
changes were accompanied by modified mem-
which has a concentration of unesterified choles-
brane bulk fluidity.
terol higher than any other tissue (23 mg/g), con-
Statins reduce the expression of the raft
taining 23% of the sterol present in the whole
marker protein flotillin. Statins directly or indi-
body pool in humans while accounting for only
rectly exert various effects on cell membrane cho-
2.1% of body weight. The relationships between
lesterol homeostasis in the CNS (Kirsch et al.
plasma cholesterol concentration and sterol
2003). A rat brain perfusion study demonstrated
metabolism in the CNS or cognitive functions in
that fluvastatin crosses blood brain barrier with
the brain remain unclear (Dietschy and Turley
permeability coefficient of 2.5 × 10 (-4) (Guillot
2004). There are reports of the effects of statins
et al. 1993). Some other statins cross the blood
on cognitive functions in patients with primary
brain barrier to a much greater extent. These
hypercholesterolemia (Gengo et al. 1995).
studies suggest that statins affect brain lipid com-
Another study suggested that lovastatin treatment
position, cell membrane and cholesterol homeo-
might cause small performance decrements on
stasis; hence CNS functions cause behavioral
neuropsychological tests of attention and psycho-
motor speeds, but in the same study no evidence
Fluvastatin usage did not affect the spatial
was found in psychological distress or substantial
memory and reference learning in the rats in our
cognitive function alterations in hypercholesterol-
study. Even though a study indicated that
emic adults. Another study, comprising of 308
Atorvastatin treatment promoted the restoration of
hypercholesterolemic adults, suggested that there
spatial memory function in rats with traumatic
are minor decrements in cognitive functioning
brain injury, this might be a result of increased
with simvastatin treatment (Muldoon et al. 2000,
blood flow after the injury (Lu et al. 2004).
2004). However, two cases were reported to have
Existing reports are not sufficient to make clear
significant temporary cognitive impairment relat-
conclusions about the memory effects of the statin
ed to statin therapy (King et al. 2003). A review
usage in animals and humans. The effects of
of a literature study about statin-associated
statin administration on memory and learning in
memory loss of 60 cases using Med-Watch drug
rats are less than clear.
surveillance system of the Food and Drug
Our results on daily activity tests that the flu-
Administration between November 1997 and
vastatin group made less movements during the
February 2002 was presented by Wagstaff et al.
course of time compared to the control group sug-
(2003). Severe irritability and aggression with
gested that these results may be related to a mus-
statin usage in six patients were also reported by
cle injury caused by fluvastatin use. It was shown
Golomb et al. (2004a). There is an ongoing dou-
that fluvastatin inhibits Rho/Rho-kinase signalling
ble-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial with
and causes disruption of the actin cytoskeleton in
selected 1,000 non-cardiac patients that will be
rat smooth muscle cultures (Kato et al. 2004). In
available for assessing the impact of the statins in
some cases, side effects occur in skeletal muscle,
cognitive functions, personality, sleep, behavioral
including myositis or even rhabdomyolysis.
irritability, and blood serotonin levels correlated
Simvastatin causes apoptosis in differentiated
Behavioral Effects of Fluvastatin in Rats
human skeletal muscle cells (Sacher et al. 2005).
the noradrenergic fibers of the locus coeruleus,
The potential role of the depleted isoprenoid pool
and serotonergic fibers from the median raphe
in the pathophysiology of statin myopathy is
(Gray 1994).
discussed by Baker (2005).
According to Gray (1990), the hippocampus
If a muscle injury was present with fluvas-
is an important contributor to the BIS, causing the
tatin treatment, we would expect less activity and
individual to stop and attend to environmental
shorter walking time from the fluvastatin group in
cues forwarded from other brain structures.
the rotarod tests. But the fluvastatin group did not
Information processed by the hippocampus is
show this type of behavior and preferred to stay
joined in the hypothalamus with information
and walk on rotarod for periods of longer time.
processed by a brain structure called the septum.
These results suggest that fluvastatin may alter
The hypothalamus generates behavioral respons-
psychomotor behavior at higher cortical or spinal
es. The role of the amygdala in this system is to
levels. With applied rotarod device tests, anxiety
give emotional response based on conditioning to
may be elicited by stimuli associated with the
stimuli that predict the occurrence of reinforce-
omission or loss of reward. Since neither reward
ment. The BIS is in part moderated through
nor a punisment is employed during tests, this
the action of the neurotransmitter Gamma-
might invoke a state of anxiety state in the sub-
aminobutyric acid (GABA) on the ascending
jects and activate the Behavioral Inhibition
noradrenergic and serotonergic pathways to the
System (BIS) proposed by Gray (1991).
hippocampus. GABA is mostly inhibitory and it
The Behavioral Approach System (BAS)
is involved in the regulation of behavior. Rats
activates approach behaviors in response to cues
demonstrated a weakened emotional response
for reward or non-punishment (Gray 1994). It
after the hippocampus was lesioned, and GABA
may be neurophysiologically associated with the
increases in the nucleus accumbens were blocked
motor programming systems in the CNS. The
during the acquisition and expression of a condi-
key components are the basal ganglia, the dopa-
tioned emotional response (Saul'skaia and
minergic fibers that ascend from the mesencepha-
Gorbachevskaia 1998). Increases in GABA levels
lon to innervate the basal ganglia, thalamic nuclei
in the amygdala mitigate the intensity of anxiety.
closely linked to the basal ganglia and similarly
GABAergic neurons are modulated by dopami-
neocortical areas such as motor, sensorimotor, and
nergic and noradrenergic input from the nucleus
prefrontal cortex which are also closely linked to
accumbens (Steiniger-Brach and Kretchmer
the basal ganglia. Dopamine is known to play an
2005). Once BIS was activated, predictions were
essential moderating role in the functioning of the
made based on memories of previous experience
BAS (Depue and Iacono 1989).
provided by the prefrontal cortex. The memory-
BIS is the main cortical behavioral inhibitory
based predictions were then compared with events
system. Signals of punishment, non-reward,
occurring in the present. Incongruence between
novel stimuli, and innate fear stimuli lead to
the prediction and reality results in behavioral
behavioral inhibition, an increment in tense arous-
inhibition and increased physical arousal and
al, and increased attention. The BIS may be con-
attention, the individual searches for more infor-
sidered both as a cognitive and physiological sys-
mation (Gray 1994).
tem (Fowles 1988; Gray 1991). Cognitively, the
Dopamine and acetylcholine were used in
role of the BIS is to compare the current state of
the nucleus accumbens according to Gray (1990).
the world with expectations and to inhibit and
In the BAS the nucleus accumbens is involved in
modify behavior that leads to deviations from
general incentive motivation for approach behav-
expectation. Physiologically, the comparator
ior. The path between the nucleus accumbens and
function of the BIS is associated with the septo-
the ventral tegmental area of the brain (where
hippocampal system. Input to this system comes
dopamine is present) allows motive to be translat-
from the prefrontal cortex output flows through
ed to behavior through dopaminergic relay of
S.H. Baytan et al.
input to the prefrontal cortex as presented by Lee
be either or both results of loss of motor cortical
et al. (1998).
neurons and possible muscle damage by high
It has been reported that high doses of simv-
dose of fluvastatin treatment. This is caused by
astatin upregulate dopamine D1 and D2 receptor
apoptosis in differentiated human skeletal muscle
expression in the rat prefrontal cortex by a possi-
cells (Sacher et al. 2005) or statin affected NMDA
ble involvement of endothelial nitric oxide syn-
receptor functions. Local dopaminergic modula-
thase (Wang et al. 2005). Protective effects of
tion of the motor activity induced by NMDA
fluvastatin from the 1-methyl-4-phenylpyridine
receptor stimulation in the ventral hippocampus
induced hydroxyl radical generation in the rat
was shown by Gimenez-Llort (2002).
striatum in cultured cells is reported (Obata and
Our results suggest that fluvastatin might
Yamanaka 2000).
affect psychomotor performance and daily activi-
From our results we can speculate that fluv-
ty in rats, but not the spatial memory. We con-
astatin treated rats ran longer times on the rotarod
clude that unreduced pscyhomotor performance in
caused by either prefrontal cortical neuron loss
the fluvastatin group in contrast to the control
from the BIS related prefrontal association areas
group may be associated with altered BIS and
resulted in decreased BIS activity or increased
BAS under the effects of the fluvastatin in the
BAS activity lead by increased cortical dopami-
brain. Daily activity changes are suggested to be
nergic activity, which might be the cause of the
results of the reduced dopaminergic activity along
decreased inhibited reaction towards a stressful
with cortical lipid profile changes or with possible
stimulus. There is a possibility that these mecha-
muscle damage of the fluvastatin. Further behav-
nisms might have been worked together.
ioral and receptor level neural studies are needed
BAS or BIS is not in effect in daily activity
to explain these effects.
evaluations, because there is no reward or punish-
ment stimulus involved in this type of test.
Atorvastatin attenuated the glutamate-induced
Special thanks to Ozturk Medical Inc., Trab-
increase of intracellular calcium, which was asso-
zon-Turkey for the financial support of this study.
ciated with a modulation of N-methyl-D-asparate
We also like to thank Mr. Kemal Uzun for his
(NMDA) receptor functions (Boesel et al. 2005).
enormous helpful efforts in the studies.
Also it is shown that HMG-CoA reductase inhibi-
tion causes a profound reduction of neurite length,
neurite loss and neuron death in undifferentiated
Baker, S.K. (2005) Molecular clues into the pathogenesis of
rat cortical neurons (Schulz et al. 2004). So, it
statin-mediated muscle toxicity.
Muscle Nerve,
31,
could be expected that modulation of NMDA
Barnes, C.A. (1979) Memory deficits associated with senes-
dependent glutamate receptors and cortical neuron
cence: a neurophysiological and behavioral study in the rat.
deaths and neurite losses can lead a functional
J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol.,
93, 74-104.
Bjorkhem, I. & Meaney, S. (2004) Brain cholesterol: long
change in all those systems, including behavioral
secret life behind a barrier.
Arterioscler. Thromb. Vasc.
response systems.
Biol.,
24, 806-815.
Our results have shown that stereotypical
Boesel, J., Gandor, F., Harms, C., Synowitz, M., Harms, U.,
Djoufack, P.C., Megow, D., Dirnagl, U., Hortnagl, H., Fink,
and vertical movements of daily activities were
K.B. & Endres, M. (2005) Neuroprotective effects of
less in the fluvastatin group than the control
atorvastatin against glutamate-induced excitotoxicity in
group. But ambulatory movements and distances
primary cortical neurones.
J. Neurochem.,
92, 1386-1398.
Col ins, R., Armitage, J., Parish, S., Sleight, P. & Peto, R. (2004)
taken in a day is decreased within the fluvastatin
Effects of cholesterol-lowering with simvastatin on stroke
group during the course of time. Ambulatory
and other major vascular events in 20536 people with cere-
brovascular disease or other high-risk conditions.
Lancet,
movements and distances were not found differ-
ent between the control and fluvastatin groups,
De Angelis, G. (2004) The influence of statin characteristics on
with the exception of the 3rd week comparison
their safety and tolerability.
Int. J. Clin. Pract.,
58,
between the groups. Animal reduced activity can
Depue, R.A. & Iacono, W.G. (1989) Neurobehavioral aspects
Behavioral Effects of Fluvastatin in Rats
of affective disorders.
Annu. Rev. Psychol.,
40, 457-492.
lipoprotein cholesterol.
Am. J. Cardiol.,
96, 24F-33F.
Dietschy, J.M. & Turley, S.D. (2004) Thematic Review Series:
Lu, D., Mahmood, A., Goussev, A., Schallert, T., Qu, C., Zhang,
Brain Lipids. Cholesterol metabolism in the central ner-
Z.G., Li, Y., Lu, M. & Chopp, M. (2004) Atorvastatin
vous system during early development and in the mature
reduction of intravascular thrombosis, increase in cerebral
animal.
J. Lipid Res.,
45, 1375-1397.
microvascular patency and integrity, and enhancement of
Fowles, D.C. (1988) Psychophysiology and psychopathology:
spatial learning in rats subjected to traumatic brain injury.
J.
A motivational approach.
Psychophysiology,
25, 373-391.
Neurosurg.,
101, 813-821.
Gengo, F., Cwudzinski, D., Kinkel, P., Block, G., Stauffer, L. &
Maron, D.J., Fazio, S. & Linton, M.F. (2000) Current
Lines, C. (1995) Effects of treatment with lovastatin and
perspectives on statins.
Circulation,
101, 207-213.
pravastatin on daytime cognitive performance.
Clin.
Mauch, D.H., Nagler, K., Schumacher, S., Goritz, C., Muller,
Cardiol.,
18, 209-214.
E.C., Otto, A. & Pfrieger, F.W. (2001) CNS synaptogenesis
Gimenez-Llort, L., Wang, F., Ogren, S.O. & Ferre, S. (2002)
promoted by glia-derived cholesterol.
Science,
294,
Local dopaminergic modulation of the motor activity
induced by N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor stimulation in
Muldoon, M.F., Barger, S.D., Ryan, C.M., Flory, J.D.,
the ventral hippocampus.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 26,
Lehoczky, J.P., Matthews, K.A. & Manuck, S.B. (2000)
Effects of lovastatin on cognitive function and psychologi-
Golomb, B.A., Kane, T. & Dimsdale, J.E. (2004a) Severe irri-
cal well-being.
Am. J. Med.,
108, 538-546.
tability associated with statin cholesterol-lowering drugs.
Muldoon, M.F., Ryan, C.M., Sereika, S.M., Flory, J.D. &
QJM, 97, 229-235.
Manuck, S.B. (2004) Randomized trial of the effects of
Golomb, B.A., Criqui, M.H., White, H.L. & Dimsdale, J.E.
simvastatin on cognitive functioning in hypercholester-
(2004b) The UCSD Statin Study: a randomized controlled
olemic adults.
Am. J. Med.,
117, 823-829.
trial assessing the impact of statins on selected noncardiac
Norton, W.T. & Cammer, W. (1984) Isolation and characteriza-
outcomes.
Control. Clin. Trials.,
25, 178-202.
tion of myelin. In:
Myelin, edited by P. Morell, Vol. 2,
Gray, J.A. (1990) Brain systems that mediate both emotion and
Plenum Press, New York, pp. 147-180.
cognition.
Cognition and Emotion,
4, 269-288.
Obata, T. & Yamanaka, Y. (2000) Protective effect of
Gray, J.A. (1991) Neural systems, emotion, and personality.
fluvastatin, a new inhibitor of 3-hydroxy-3-methylglutaryl
In:
Ne
urobiology of Learning, Emotion, and Affect, edited
coenzyme A reductase, on MPP(+)-induced hydroxyl
by J. Madden, Raven Press, NewYork, pp. 273-306.
radical in the rat striatum.
Brain Res.,
860, 166-169.
Gray, J.A. (1994) Framework for a taxonomy of psychiatric
Parale, G.P., Bahetti N.N., Kulkarni, P.M. & Panchhal, N.V.
disorder. In:
Emotions: Essays on Emotion Theory, edited
(2006) Effects of atorvastatin on higher functions.
Eur. J.
by S.H.M. van Goozen, N.E. van de Poll & J. Sergeant,
Clin. Pharmacol.,
27, 425-446.
Hillsdale Erlbaum, New Jersey, pp. 29-59.
Sacher, J., Weigl, L., Werner, M., Szegedi, C. & Hohenegger, M.
Guillot, F., Misslin, P. & Lemaire, M. (1993) Comparison of
(2005) Delineation of myotoxicity induced by 3-hydroxy-
fluvastatin and lovastatin blood-brain barrier transfer using
3-methylglutaryl CoA reductase inhibitors in human skeletal
in vitro and in vivo methods.
J. Cardiovasc. Pharmacol.,
muscle cells.
J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther., 314, 1032-1041.
21, 339-346.
Saul'skaia, N.B. & Gorbachevskaia, A.I. (1998) Participation
Johnson-Anuna, L.N., Eckert, G.P., Keller, J.H., Igbavboa, U.,
of the hippocampus in regulation of GABA release in the
Franke, C., Fechner, T., Schubert-Zsilavecz, M., Karas, M.,
nucleus accumbens during emotional conditioning.
Ross
Muller, W.E. & Wood, W.G. (2005) Chronic administra-
Fiziol. Zh. Im. I. M. Sechenova.,
84, 43-49.
tion of statins alters multiple gene expression patterns in
Schaefer, E.J., McNamara, J.R., Tayler, T., Daly, J.A., Gleason,
mouse cerebral cortex.
J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther.,
312,
J.L., Seman, L.J., Ferrari, A. & Rubenstein, J.J. (2004)
Comparisons of effects of statins (atorvastatin, fluvastatin,
Kato, T., Hashikabe, H., Iwata, C., Akimoto, K. & Hattori, Y.
lovastatin, pravastatin, and simvastatin) on fasting and
(2004) Statin blocks Rho/Rho-kinase signalling and dis-
postprandial lipoproteins in patients with coronary heart
rupts the actin cytoskeleton: relationship to enhancement of
disease versus control subjects.
Am. J. Cardiol.,
93, 31-39.
LPS-mediated nitric oxide synthesis in vascular smooth
Schulz, J.G., Bosel, J., Stoeckel, M., Megow, D., Dirnagl, U. &
muscle cells.
Biochim. Biophys. Acta,
1689, 267-272.
Endres, M. (2004) HMG-CoA reductase inhibition causes
King, D.S., Wilburn, A.J., Wofford, M.R., Harrell, T.K.,
neurite loss by interfering with geranylgeranylpyrophospha
Lindley, B.J. & Jones, D.W. (2003) Cognitive impairment
te synthesis.
J. Neurochem.,
89, 24-32.
associated with atorvastatin and simvastatin.
Pharmaco-
Snipes, G.J. & Suter, U. (1997) Cholesterol and Myelin. In:
therapy,
23, 1663-1667.
Subcellular Biochemistry, edited by R. Bittman, Vol. 28,
Kirsch, C., Eckert, G.P. & Mueller, W.E. (2003) Statin effects
Plenum Press, New York, pp. 173-204.
on cholesterol micro-domains in brain plasma membranes.
Steiniger-Brach, B. & Kretschmer, B.D. (2005) Different
Biochem. Pharmacol.,
65, 843-856.
function of pedunculopontine GABA and glutamate
Lee, R.S., Koob, G.F. & Henriksen, S.J. (1998) Electrophysio-
receptors in nucleus accumbens dopamine, peduncu-
logical responses of nucleus accumbens neurons to novelty
lopontine glutamate and operant discriminative behavior.
stimuli and exploratory behavior in the awake, unrestrained
Eur. J. Neurosci.,
22, 1720-1730.
rat.
Brain Res.,
20, 317-322.
Turner, R.J., Van den Heuvel, C. & Vink, R. (2004) Amiloride
Liao, J.K. (2002) Lipid lowering: the role of statins in vascular
increases neuronal damage after traumatic brain injury in
protection.
Int. J. Cardiol.,
86, 5-18.
rats.
J. Am. Coll. Nutr.,
23, 534S-537S.
Liao, J.K. (2004) Statins: potent vascular anti-inflammatory
Vecka, M., Tvrzicka, E., Stankova, B., Novak, F., Novakova, O.
agents.
Int. J. Clin. Pract. Suppl.,
143, 41-48.
& Zak, A. (2004) Hypolipidemic drugs can change the
Liao, J.K. (2005) Effects of statins on 3-hydroxy-3-methylglu-
composition of rat brain lipids.
Tohoku J. Exp. Med.,
204,
taryl coenzyme a reductase inhibition beyond low-density
S.H. Baytan et al.
Wagstaff, L.R., Mitton, M.W., Arvik, B.M. & Doraiswamy, P.M.
doses of simvastatin upregulate dopamine D1 and D2
(2003) Statin-associated memory loss: analysis of 60 case
receptor expression in the rat prefrontal cortex: possible
reports and review of the literature.
Pharmacotherapy,
23,
involvement of endothelial nitric oxide synthase.
Br. J.
Pharmacol.,
144, 933-939.
Wang, Q., Ting, W.L., Yang, H. & Wong, P.T. (2005) High
Source: http://ceng2.ktu.edu.tr/~ekinci/pdf/2006/tib06.pdf
Guidance for commissioners of rehabilitation services for people with complex mental health needs 1 Joint Commissioning Panel for Mental Healthwww.jcpmh.info Guidance for commissioners of rehabilitation services for people with complex mental health needs Joint Commissioning Panel for Mental Healthwww.jcpmh.info 2 Practical Mental Health Commissioning Ten key messages
Minnesota Chapter of the American Academy of Pediatrics Pediatric Abstract and Poster Competition Hilton Hotel- MOA/MSP Bloomington, MN Medical Student Roma Patel What does that lab really mean? MSV Matthew Armfield, Introduction: Ovarian masses in the pre-pubertal population are concerning